Skip to main content
Sleep logoLink to Sleep
. 2013 Jul 1;36(7):1077–1084. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2814

Behavioral Sleep Problems and their Potential Impact on Developing Executive Function in Children

Kathryn Turnbull 1,, Graham J Reid 1,2,3,4, J Bruce Morton 1
PMCID: PMC3669074  PMID: 23814345

Abstract

Bedtime resistance and night waking are common sleep problems throughout childhood, especially in the early years. These sleep problems may lead to difficulties in neurobehavioral functioning, but most research into childhood sleep problems has not emphasized the importance of the developmental context in which disruptions in neurobehavioral and daytime functioning occur. We review the development of sleep as well as executive functioning (EF) in childhood and suggest that EF may be particularly vulnerable to the effects of these common childhood sleep problems because of its prolonged course of maturation. Behavioral problems associated with common sleep problems suggest poor self-regulation in the context of sleep loss, and developing EF skills play important roles in self-regulation. A research agenda that considers a developmental approach to sleep and sleep problems in the context of childhood EF performance is outlined to promote future research in this area.

Citation:

Turnbull K; Reid GJ; Morton JB. Behavioral sleep problems and their potential impact on developing executive function in children. SLEEP 2013;36(7):1077-1084.

Keywords: Bedtime problems, night wakings, behavioral insomnia of childhood, child development, neurobehavioral functioning

INTRODUCTION

Behavioral sleep problems are the most common sleep problems in childhood within the general population. Between 15% and 30% of 2- to 5-year-old children experience regular difficulties falling asleep (i.e., bedtime problems) or sleeping through the night (i.e., night waking).14 Fewer school-aged children experience behavioral sleep problems compared to preschool children, but these problems still occur in 11% to 15% of school-age children (6-12 years).5,6 Behavioral sleep problems result in reduced sleep duration and/or sleep fragmentation, leading to varying degrees of chronic sleep restriction. As we learn more about children's sleep habits and sleep problems through population-based studies,2,711 there is a need to determine how these prevalent sleep disruptions might affect children during the course of development. Given that a substantial proportion of children experience behavioral sleep problems at some point, there is a need to better understand the impact of sleep restriction and fragmentation on children's neurobehavioral functioning.

The impact of sleep restriction on neurobehavioral functioning in adults has been extensively examined,1214 but few studies have been conducted with children. The effects of sleep restriction and sleep problems may vary with age, because neurological functioning15,16 and sleep needs8 change considerably during child development. We examine research from cognitive developmental psychology and neuroscience, as well as pediatric sleep research to illustrate how neurobehavioral skills with a protracted developmental course (specifically executive functioning skills) could be negatively influenced by behavioral sleep problems.

PROBLEMATIC SLEEP IN EARLY CHILDHOOD: BEDTIME RESISTANCE AND NIGHT WAKING

Children resist going to bed for a variety of reasons: because they wish to engage in other preferred activities, because they do not feel tired, because they have nighttime fears that make them frightened of going to sleep alone, etc. Bedtime resistance involves active oppositional behavior on the part of the child and in turn may lead to shorter sleep duration. Night waking problems occur when children wake at night and do not fall back to sleep promptly and independently. Many young children experience transient waking periods through the night, but quickly return to sleep on their own, without intervention from parents.1719 When children wake frequently and/or for an extended period of time and require parental assistance (e.g., parental presence, soothing activities) to return to sleep, night wakings become problematic.

Correlational studies have found a relationship between parent-reported sleep problems and child psychosocial problems, including attention problems, hyperactivity, oppositional and aggressive behavior, mood problems, and anxiety.1,11,2028 Poor school functioning and lower cognitive performance have also been associated with sleep problems in children, suggesting that sleep disruptions can impair cognitive processes.2931 Experimental studies that have assigned school-age children to restricted versus extended sleep schedules have confirmed that even limited sleep deficits (e.g., 1 h/day for 3 days) cause measurable deficits in cognitive functioning measures32 and child behavior.33 Thus, research strongly suggests that healthy sleep plays an important role in children's daytime functioning. However, few studies point to specific domains of functioning that are compromised when sleep is disrupted. Recently, studies have included standardized measures of attention and memory, as a means of clarifying more specific neurobehavioral impacts of sleep problems in school-aged children34 and toddlers.35

Children with conditions that involve behavior problems and difficulty with self-regulation, notably attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autistic spectrum disorder (ASD), present with behavioral sleep problems more frequently than children within the general population, particularly daytime sleepiness (ADHD) and shorter overall sleep duration, reflecting difficulty settling to sleep, night waking, and early morning waking (ASD).3640 Children with ADHD have been found to experience more fatigue and sleepiness during the day compared to samples of children without ADHD,41,42 although not all studies have found significant differences in the amount of sleep obtained by children with ADHD and typical comparison groups.38,43 Some studies of sleep problems in children with ADHD and ASD have found sleep disruptions other than behavioral sleep problems (i.e., sleep disordered breathing and restless leg syndrome) to be more frequent in children with ASD and ADHD.44 There is evidence that sleep fragmentation and sleep restriction in children with ADHD is related to poorer performance on measures of sustained attention,45,46 but few treatment studies have measured changes or improvements in the behavior of children with ADHD or ASD as a result of sleep treatments. A recent review of sleep treatment studies in children with ADHD, ASD, and other chronic conditions (e.g., visual impairment, intellectual/developmental disability) highlights the methodological weaknesses of previous sleep treatment studies conducted with children in special populations, including children with ADHD and ASD.47

There has been some research on the effects of sleep treatment to improve daytime functioning in typical children. The purpose of sleep treatment is to consolidate sleep at night and to reduce bedtime delays.4851 Not only is behavioral treatment of common sleep problems effective, but improvements in daytime behavior are observed in young children whose sleep problems are successfully treated.52 However, many sleep treatment studies report only whether children's sleep improves as a result of treatment without assessing other behavioral outcomes or daytime functioning. There are now many published studies of behavioral interventions for sleep in children under five years of age,50,5355 and similar interventions are successful with older children.56

The severity of bedtime and night waking problems in children have typically been described in terms of overall disruptions of sleep (i.e., times waking per night, sleep duration) and the duration of the problem (i.e., months, years). At this time, clinicians and researchers do not agree on the point at which transient disturbances in children's sleep become significant enough to constitute a sleep disorder,19 although suggested definitions for research purposes have recently been proposed.57 In practice, childhood behavioral sleep problems are most often identified and treated when a parent finds their own sleep or evening activities significantly disrupted. Therefore, clinical presentation of childhood sleep problems depends more on the effect of the child's sleep problems on parents and families than on the effect of these sleep problems on the child. A focus on neurobehavioral outcomes in the context of sleep problems will help to inform both clinical and research definitions of what constitutes a mild versus clinically significant sleep problem in terms of the possible effects that the sleep problem will have on the child.

SLEEP DEVELOPMENT

The amount of sleep that children receive decreases through the course of childhood, reflecting a gradual decrease in the need for sleep with age.7,8 During sleep, children experience REM sleep alternating with NREM or slow wave periods of sleep. Like adults, toddlers (age 1-2 years) and children begin sleep in stage 1 of NREM sleep and proceed through stages 2-4, with each progressive stage involving slower, more synchronous electroencephalographic (EEG) activity. Interspersed with periods of NREM sleep are periods of REM sleep, which are characterized by EEG activity that resembles wakefulness. Throughout the night, children cycle through these different sleep states, but depending on their age they spend more or less time in certain states of sleep than others. Most notably, younger children spend a much higher proportion of their time asleep in REM sleep compared to older children and adults.58,59 As children age, they also experience a decreased number of sleep cycles over the course of a night, and sleep becomes more efficient, so that periods of waking from sleep at night become less common with age.58 Overall, differences in sleep architecture over the course of childhood are thought to reflect differences in sleep need across different points in ongoing neurological development, especially the rapid decrease in REM sleep in the first few years of life followed by its slow decline through childhood and adolescence.60,61

Changes in normative sleep during childhood are often studied separately from the incidence and impact of behavioral sleep problems. Cross-sectional studies examining normative sleep schedules in children often use parent report of whether the child has a “sleep problem”4,62 without assessing the nature of the problem. Night waking frequency tends to peak in young children around 1 and 2 years of age.2,4,24 Delayed sleep onset and bedtime resistance tend to be less common than night waking in 1- and 2-year-olds, increase in prevalence in 3- and 4-year-olds, and decline in frequency as children approach school age.2 The tendency of behavioral sleep problems to decline in prevalence as children reach school age suggests that these problems might be self-limiting in some children. However, longitudinal research and follow-up studies also indicate that sleep problems and disorders can emerge and resolve at different ages, while in some children they persist with the presence of a sleep problem in infancy being a strong predictor of sleep problems in early childhood.18,63,64 It is noteworthy that sleep problems are far more common during early childhood compared to later childhood, at the same period when there could be a greater need for sleep to support ongoing neurological development.

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING IN CHILDHOOD

Behavioral sleep problems are most prevalent at the time in development at which children's self-regulatory abilities undergo rapid age-related change. These self-regulatory abilities—“executive functions”—are a set of processes that guide perception, emotion, and action, especially in situations marked by distraction, novelty, or stress.65 Young parents, for example, typically leave the office at the end of the day and drive straight to pick up their children from school. On some occasions though, they may have to first stop and buy food for dinner and then pick up their children. The ability to adjust habitual patterns of behavior in this way draws on the executive functions—processes that help us to formulate and maintain plans while resisting overlearned habits and powerful emotions.

Executive functions (EF) are profoundly underdeveloped in preschool-aged children, as illustrated by performance in a variety of simple laboratory tasks. For example, in a multi-location search task, 2-year-old children retrieve a small reward hidden in 1 of 3 different hiding locations.66 On the first trial, children do very well, easily finding the hidden reward. However, in later trials, when the reward is hidden at a new location, children struggle by repeatedly (and erroneously) searching for the reward at the first location. And in the Dimensional Change Card Sort task,67 3-year-old children sort cards one way (e.g., by shape) and then are instructed to switch and sort the same cards in a new way (e.g., by color). Again, children do well on initial trials, accurately sorting cards by their shape, but then struggle when the sorting rule changes to color. Instead of switching to the new rules, they persist in using the old rules. Thus, children's difficulties pertain to the regulatory demands of the task. They have little trouble learning to find hidden rewards or sort cards using simple rules. Instead, they struggle when asked to flexibly override what they first learned. Other tasks used to study the development of executive control include the Delay of Gratification procedure. In this task, children are given the option of enjoying a small immediate reward or waiting for a larger preferred reward. These tasks assess self-regulation under conditions in which motivation is enhanced and emotions are aroused.68,69 Strong emotional responses may make it more difficult to use EF skills to act in socially appropriate ways,70 and may lead children to act in ways they know are “wrong” (e.g., a child may fail to wait his/her turn in a game when excited, even when he/she knows the rule and will wait in other situations).

EF shows continued development into early adulthood, as illustrated by better performance in rule-use tasks,71 and a greater capacity to withstand delays with increasing age. These protracted cognitive developmental changes are thought to be associated with age-related changes in the structural and functional organization of lateral prefrontal cortex.72 By almost any anatomical or functional metric, including cortical thickness,73 regional volume,74 myelination,75 resting metabolism,76 and rate of synaptogenesis,77 lateral prefrontal cortex is among the slowest-developing regions of cortex. These ongoing changes in structure occur in a region of the brain that is well-known from adult neuroimaging and neuropsychology studies to be linked with EF.78 Lateral prefrontal cortex tends, for example, to be more active in trials that require response inhibition and mental flexibility compared to trials that do not.79 As well, damage or dysfunction in lateral prefrontal cortex leads to pronounced performance deficits in inhibitory control80 and mental flexibility.81 The fact that behavioral sleep problems occur with great frequency at a point in development in which the capacity for behavioral self-regulation and associated neural circuitry undergo rapid age-related change calls for closer consideration of how children's sleep and their neurobehavioral development might be related.

BRINGING CHILD SLEEP INTO THE CONTEXT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING DEVELOPMENT

There has been relatively little attention to the relationship between EF performance and child sleep. Only one study of very young children (12 to 26 months old)35 has specifically considered the ongoing development of children's self-regulation skills in the context of sleep. Other researchers have previously proposed that poor sleep may specifically affect EF because of noticeable declines in performance on EF tasks and/ or self-regulation in children and adults with poor sleep.8284 Experimental sleep restriction studies in adults (e.g., limiting sleep to 4 h/night) have also reported evidence of difficulties in planning and organizing behavior,12 though not all adults studies have reported decreased performance on basic EF functions, such as working memory.14 Sleep problems that occur in ADHD and in ASD may be relevant to the connection between sleep problems and poor EF performance. In both ADHD and ASD, EF skills tend to develop more slowly compared to typically developing children, and EF deficits are evident among children and adolescents with these diagnoses.8588 Children with ADHD have difficulty with EF skills such as organization and behavioral inhibition, and children with ASD show reduced performance in attention and EF domains, beyond the effects of general intellectual functioning.89,90 It is notable that these conditions, which include core deficits in EF, frequently co-occur with sleep problems. However, the available literature on sleep treatment studies for children with ASD and ADHD is not of sufficient quality to determine whether a causal relationship between improved sleep and improved EF might exist in these conditions.47 Neurological differences that characterize ASD and/or ADHD might also reduce the extent to which relationships between sleep and EF functioning can be applied to typically developing children with sleep problems. Further research examining the relationship between behavioral sleep problems and EF functioning in children with ADHD or ASD could help to inform our understanding of the relationship between sleep and EF functioning as well as providing information relevant to the treatment of sleep problems in the management of these disorders.

If sleep problems lead to EF difficulties in typically developing children, it could help to account for the association between poor daytime functioning and sleep problems. This may occur in 4 ways:

First, behavioral sleep problems may interfere with children's ability to implement developing EF skills in daily life. Specifically, sleep problems and short sleep duration contribute to fatigue; in turn, lower levels of energy would impair the ability to implement EF skills. The execution of novel skills and behaviors requires greater intentionality, and likely more energy, compared to overlearned, well-established behaviors. As young children's EF skills are undergoing development they may be particularly influenced and impaired by fatigue. This would have consequences for behavioral and emotional regulation, leading to increased negative emotionality, difficulty transitioning between different daytime tasks, oppositional behavior, and other daytime activities requiring application of EF skills. Recent research confirms that EF skills measured in standardized laboratory conditions relate strongly to behavior problems among children aged 2 to 6 years.91,92 The consistent reports of increased behavior problems in the context of sleep difficul-ties and sleep restriction1,11,2028,33 suggest that poor sleep quality interferes with children's ability to regulate their behavior. The disruption of EF skills, such as working memory, would also explain difficulties in school performance (e.g., lower reported grades31 and greater failure to meet grade requirements) among children who receive less sleep.2931 If children are not able to maintain relevant information in mind when presented with novel information in a school setting, their overall school performance would be expected to suffer.

Second, there might be a concurrent causal association between behavioral sleep problems and EF early in development. An underdeveloped capacity for self-regulation could make it difficult for children to establish consistent sleep patterns. This could be evidenced either at bedtime, when children need to adhere to a routine and avoid distractions, or at night, when, having awoken, they need to soothe themselves back to sleep. Regardless of when such sleep issues occur, mitigating potential sleep disruption would require a capacity for cognitive and behavioral self-regulation, something which is underdeveloped in young children. From this perspective, maturation and development of EF might account for the decrease in prevalence of behavioral sleep problems from infancy through to elementary school.

Third, sleep disturbance might adversely impact EF in individuals of all ages, but have a more pronounced effect on younger as compared to older children, given that executive functioning develops slowly. This could, in turn, undermine the stability of sleep-related behaviors to the extent that, in young children, sleep is dependent on self-regulation. In essence then, there could be a dynamic, concurrent association between sleep disruption and cognitive-behavioral dysregulation early in development. Namely, disrupted sleep compromises children's executive functioning, which in turn, leads to further sleep disruption.

Fourth, a more speculative possibility is that sleep disruption in early childhood is longitudinally associated with decrements in later EF. Given that EF and its associated neural circuitry undergoes rapid development between 2 and 5 years, and that sleep plays a critical restorative role in brain functioning, disrupted sleep early in development could have negative longitudinal consequences for the development of EF. The possibility that sleep reduction at certain ages might have lasting impacts on cognitive performance has been raised by certain pediatric sleep researchers.30

Ultimately, the relationship between EF and sleep problems should be empirically studied at the neurological level. Suggested neurological effects of poor sleep have ranged from decreased neuroplasticity, reducing the degree to which neurons may respond to experiential output and learning, to loss of function in neurons.61,9395 Neuroplasticity can be observed behaviorally as the rapid ability with which children can learn certain skills with limited exposure compared with adults, and research in the early neurological development of specific systems has generally found increased neuroplasticity to be present at earlier ages and stages of development.96 Cognitive and neurological development from birth through adolescence is characterized by learning and mastering behavior relevant to social, cognitive, linguistic, and emotional regulation domains. If sleep disruption is a process that can interfere with neuro-plasticity, it might have more lasting and impairing effects on the developing neurological systems supporting EF, which take years to mature.

An important feature of the proposed relationships between EF and behavioral sleep problems is the focus on understanding neurobehavioral effects of sleep in a developmental context. Interpreting the effects of sleep problems in children requires a model that takes into account ongoing changes in neurological development. It is also important to note individual variations in the timing of how EF skills develop normatively, and how EF maps on to neurological maturation in childhood. A developmental context stresses the importance of individual differences.97 As such, our approach draws attention to why and how sleep problems will differentially affect children. Similar levels of severity and frequency of sleep problems may affect individual children differently. This variation in the impact of sleep disruptions could be due to differences in how well children are able to execute EF functions under optimal conditions, as a function of both their development and individual characteristics. In other words, two children who have similar levels of severity and frequency of sleep problems may evidence varying levels of impairment in their daytime functioning due to variability in the development of their EF skills.

A developmental approach also recognizes risk and resilience factors.97 Although we do not deal with the myriad potential influences on, and moderators of, the relationship between sleep and daytime functioning, we acknowledge these factors exist and are important to consider. While the basic pathway between sleep problems and EF abilities may be similar for all children, other intervening factors, such as the presence of additional environmental stressors (e.g., family stress, parents' marital conflict), or comorbid conditions (e.g., ADHD) could alter the expression of the behavioral problems in the context of sleep disruption.

A RESEARCH AGENDA FOR STUDYING EF AND SLEEP IN CHILDREN

The associations between behavioral sleep problems and EF in early development discussed above call for closer empirical examination: we suggest a number of research directions. 1) Include developmentally appropriate measures of EF that are sensitive to a range of EF abilities when investigating the effects of sleep problems in children. There are several methods to measure EF-based skills in children between the ages of 2 and 7 years.98 Beyond this period, standardized tasks also exist for the measurement of EF skills in older children.99 Two aspects of EF should be measured. First, there are EF measures that assess more cognitive aspects of EF including switching attention, working memory, inhibiting automatic behavior in accordance with a rule, and performing tasks that include conflicting information. Second, other measures involve emotional and motivational processes along with cognition, notably delay of gratification tasks and “gambling” tasks.69,100 Both cognitive and emotional/motivational EF skills could be compromised, perhaps differentially, due to sleep problems; emotional/motivational EF tasks may be more relevant to difficulties with behavior and emotion regulation. Another important element in measurement relates to the fact that many EF measures used in developmental psychology are scored as pass/fail.67 These tasks are useful for determining the average age when children typically develop certain cognitive abilities, but they are less useful for measuring differences in EF ability within groups of children of a particular age. EF measures that are scored on a continuous basis, or a battery of tasks, would be more appropriate to detect a range of ability among children of the same age such that variation in EF can be related to sleep disruptions. 2) Include measures of EF in longitudinal studies to investigate potential critical periods for the effects of sleep problems. Some researchers have suggested that sleep problems during certain ages/stages may compromise the development of particular cognitive skills.30 Longitudinal studies that assess the onset and offset of sleep problems concurrently with the development and performance in EF-related tasks could help clarify this issue. Similarly, including EF measures in treatment studies for children at different ages could test (a) if EF deficits resolve concurrently with sleep problems or sleep problems have lasting effects on EF after sleep problems resolve, and (b) if changes in EF might mediate the relationship between the secondary improvements in emotional and behavioral functioning that can occur in response to sleep interventions. This research would have important implications for both the treatment and prevention of early sleep problems. Longitudinal studies would also be important to determine the types of risk that chronic sleep problems involve with respect to the development of EF performance. For example, do early sleep problems that are resolved by the time a child is 3 or 4 years of age predict different types of EF dysfunction than sleep problems at age 5-6 years? What is the relationship between the amount of sleep disruption (i.e., short sleep duration) and amount or type of EF dysfunction? Is there a recovery period after a sleep problem is resolved, after which time a child will no longer experience difficulties in EF functioning? Such questions are highly relevant to understanding the role of sleep problems throughout childhood, and could be addressed through longitudinal studies. 3) Investigate EF performance in the context of problem-free sleep and experimentally induced sleep loss in children. There are few data on naturally existing relationships between sleep patterns and EF performance among typically developing children. One way to determine whether a causal link between sleep disruption and EF disruption exists would be assessment of EF in response to experimentally induced sleep deprivation and disruption. One recently published study using an experimental paradigm was conducted with toddlers (age 30 to 36 months old) by restricting nap duration101; a few other studies have been conducted with school-aged children.32,33 In contrast, numerous studies have examined the impact of sleep restriction and sleep deprivation in adults, both in the domain of sleep apnea and experimental sleep restriction.12,84,102,103 Random assignment of children to conditions of sleep restriction or fragmentation, resembling short-term bedtime resistance or night waking, could provide information on potential differential effects for these two types of common sleep problems on EF. If differential effects are identified, subsequent studies using polysomnography might elucidate specific parameters of sleep that are impaired or altered as a result of short sleep duration versus sleep fragmentation, which then could be linked with changes in EF.

4) Measure variables that may moderate or mediate the relationship between EF and sleep problems in children. Both EF ability and normative sleep duration show fairly wide inter-individual variability. We do not yet have reliable measures to determine if a child is obtaining optimal amounts of sleep; population studies show variability in the amount of sleep between children and within the same child at different points, consistent with variations in sleep need.4,7,8 Accounting for individual differences is necessary to investigate whether certain subgroups of children may be more vulnerable to neurocognitive disruptions in the context of sleep problems. For example, child temperament could moderate the relationship between sleep loss and neurocognitive disruptions, as suggested by research showing that sleep problems are more strongly related to externalizing behavior in children who display temperamental resistance to control.21 As there has been so little research on EF-related skills in the context of child sleep, it is difficult speculate on which individual child variables might be most relevant in the relationship between sleep and EF. 5) Design and conduct studies on the relationship between childhood sleep problems and EF function in multidisciplinary teams. Child development can be measured from many perspectives—behavioral, social, cognitive, neurological, physiological, and emotional—and each of these areas has its own history of research progress and expertise. Sleep is also a complex process that has been studied using very diverse approaches, from animal models to clinical case studies. This discussion of EF and its relationship to common behavioral sleep problems brings together evidence from the neurological, pediatric sleep, and child cognitive-developmental literatures. Basic associations between difficult behavior and child sleep problems have now been well established: it is time to advance our knowledge by drawing on the evidence of other fields to find and test connections that will inform how and why sleep problems impact child functioning.

LIMITATIONS

We have focused on the impact of common childhood sleep problems on the development and use of executive functioning skills. The protracted development of EF-based skills throughout childhood makes these skills interesting to examine in the context of sleep problems, but there are likely other pathways through which sleep restriction or fragmentation could influence neurological development and behavior throughout childhood. Reduced sleep duration has been associated with problems learning new information and committing it to memory in adults94: this would probably be true for children as well. Further research examining physiological and neurological variables in relation to sleep should include careful measurement of children's sleep, daytime behavior, and neurological/physiological measures of interest in order to propose a theory for the influence of these pathways on behavior. Ideally, assessment of sleep should involve actigraphy (motion logging) and/or video-somnography (time-lapse video taken at night) to verify parent reports of sleep. Neurological measures could include fMRI and other neuroimaging techniques that investigate changes in children's brains that occur in the context of sleep problems.

We have also focused on sleep at the level of the individual child. Sleep, like most childhood behaviors, occurs in the context of the family and develops through interaction with environmental variables, such as family organization, routines, and parenting practices. Many studies of children's sleep have identified family and environmental predictors of children's sleep patterns, such as cultural differences,104,105 family disorganization,106 parent-child relationships,107 and socioeconomic status108 that influence sleep in early childhood. It is possible that co-occurrence of behavioral sleep problems and poor EF in early development reflect the influence of other (or third) factors, such as the presence of family discord, psychopathology in the child, or other health-related issues. Given that behavioral sleep problems can be stressful for parents, child sleep problems can create additional difficulties for a family. Potential transactional effects are therefore likely to emerge, whereby a stressful family environment, or practices that parents maintain around their children's sleep, make sleep problems more likely.109,110

CONCLUSION

EF skills and their underlying mechanisms are important outcomes for researchers to measure in the context of sleep. These skills are relevant to children's everyday functioning and self-regulation, and follow an extended developmental course, potentially making these skills vulnerable to inadequate sleep. In particular, the role that optimal sleep plays in maintaining behavioral control through EF is a theoretically compelling relationship. Further evidence of the specific relationship between EF and sleep in early childhood is necessary, and could have implications for our understanding of how sleep affects children's ability to function adaptively in the course of daily life.

Advances in technology, and the accelerated pace of life in modern society are having an impact on sleep in humans of all ages, and children in industrialized countries are now sleeping less than in previous generations.111 The ubiquity of television and other screen technologies likely plays a role in the overall decrease in sleep times for children.112 Also, the necessity of sleep may be downplayed in favor of other activities. Over half of the 3-year-olds in Japan go to bed after 10 p.m. on a regular basis (versus approximately 22% of children in 1980).11 This trend towards later bedtimes and shorter sleep duration in children makes it more important than ever to examine the role that sleep plays in the maintenance of health, particularly healthy development. The amount of time that children spend asleep when cognitive skills and self-regulation are developing suggests that sleep serves a number of vital functions that support neurological development. The more we know about the specific functions of sleep in children, the more we can encourage the timely treatment of sleep problems and the safe-guarding of sleep during childhood.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

This was not an industry supported study. The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of interest.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Ms. Turnbull was supported by the Children's Health Research Institute and the Canadian Health Research Institute. Dr. Reid was supported by the Children's Health Foundation.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Lavigne JV, Arend R, Rosenbaum D, et al. Sleep and behavior problems among preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1999;20:164–9. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199906000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Petit D, Touchette E, Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Montplaisir JY. Dyssomnias and parasomnias in early childhood. Pediatrics. 2007;119:1016–25. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gregory AM, O'Connor TG. Sleep problems in childhood: A longitudinal study of developmental change and association with behavioral problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:964–71. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.National Sleep Foundation. Sleep in America Poll: Children and Sleep. National Sleep Foundation; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mindell JA, Meltzer LJ, Carskadon MA, Chervin RD. Developmental aspects of sleep hygiene: Findings from the 2004 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Med. 2009;10:771–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Owens JA, Spirito A, McGuinn M. Sleep habits and sleep disturbance in elementary school-aged children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2000;21:27–36. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200002000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jenni OG, Molinari L, Caflisch JA, Largo RH. Sleep duration from age 1 to 10 years: Variability and stability in comparison with growth. Pediatrics. 2007;120:e769–76. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Iglowstein I, Jenni OG, Molinari L, Largo RH. Sleep duration from infancy to adolescence: Reference values and generational trends. Pediatrics. 2003;111:302–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ottaviano S, Giannotti F, Cortesi F, Bruni O, Ottaviano C. Sleep characteristics in healthy children from birth to 6 years of age in the urban area of Rome. Sleep. 1996;19:1–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Scher A, Tirosh E, Jaffe M. Survey of sleep patterns of Israeli infants and young children. Int J Behav Dev. 1995;18:701–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yokomaku A, Misao K, Omoto F, et al. A study of the association between sleep habits and problematic behaviors in preschool children. Chronobiol Int. 2008;25:549–64. doi: 10.1080/07420520802261705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:519–28. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Walker MP. Cognitive consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Med. 2008;9(Suppl 1):S29–S34. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70014-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tucker AM. Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep. 2010;1:47–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Casey BJ, Galvan A, Hare TA. Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15:239–44. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Durston S, Davidson MC, Tottenham N, et al. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Dev Sci. 2006;9:1–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00454.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Touchette E, Petit D, Paquet J, et al. Factors associated with fragmented sleep at night across childhood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159:242–9. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.159.3.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zuckerman B, Stevenson J, Baily V. Sleep problems in early childhood: Continuities, predictive factors and behavioral correlates. Pediatrics. 1987;80:664–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Adair R, Bauchner H, Philipp B, Levenson S, Zuckerman B. Night waking during infancy: role of parental presence at bedtime. Pediatrics. 1991;87:500–4. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bruni O, Lo Reto F, Miano S, Ottaviano C. Daytime behavioral correlates of awakenings and bedtime resistance in preschool children. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;53:358–61. doi: 10.1016/s1567-424x(09)70181-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Goodnight JA, Bates JE, Staples AD, Petit GS, Dodge K. Temperamental resistance to control increases the association between sleep problems and externalizing behavior development. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21:39–48. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lam P, Hiscock H, Wake M. Outcomes of infant sleep problems: A longitudinal study of sleep, behavior, and maternal well-being. Pediatrics. 2003;111:e203–7. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.3.e203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Shang CY, Gau SS, Soong WT. Association between childhood sleep problems and perinatal factors, parental mental distress and behavioral problems. J Sleep Res. 2006;15:63–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2006.00492.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hiscock H, Canterford L, Ukoumunne OC, Wake M. Adverse associations of sleep problems in Australian preschoolers: National population study. Pediatrics. 2007;119:86–93. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Paavonen EJ, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Lahikainen AR. Sleep quality, duration and behavioral symptoms among 5-6-year-old children. Eur J Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;18:747–54. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0033-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Reid GJ, Hong RY, Wade TJ. The relation between common sleep problems and emotional and behavioral problems among 2- and 3-year-olds in the context of known risk factors for psychopathology. J Sleep Res. 2009;18:49–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00692.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Richman N. A community survey of characteristics of one- to two-year-olds with sleep disruptions. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1981;20:281–91. doi: 10.1016/s0002-7138(09)60989-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Coulombe J, Reid GJ, Boyle MH, Racine Y. Concurrent associations among sleep problems, indicators of inadequate sleep, psychopathology, and shared risk factors in a population-based sample of healthy Ontario children. J Pediatr Psychol. 2012;35:790–9. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ravid S, Afek I, Suraiya S, Shahar E, Pillar G. Kindergarten children's failure to qualify for first grade could result from sleep disturbances. J Child Neurol. 2009;24:816–822. doi: 10.1177/0883073808330766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Touchette E, Petit D, Seguin JR, Bovin M, Tremblay RE, Montplaisir J. Associations between sleep duration patterns and behavioral/cognitive functioning at school entry. Sleep. 2007;30:1213–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.9.1213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Meijer A. Chronic sleep reduction, functioning at school and school achievement in preadolescents. J Sleep Res. 2008;17:395–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00677.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sadeh A, Gruber R, Raviv A. The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: What a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003;74:444–55. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fallone G, Acebo C, Seifer R, Carskadon MA. Experimental restriction of sleep opportunity in children: Effects on teacher ratings. Sleep. 2005;28:1516–67. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.12.1561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sadeh A. Sleep, neurobehavioral functioning, and behavior problems in school-age children. Child Dev. 2002;73:405–17. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Bernier A, Carlson SM, Bordeleau S, Carrier J. Relations between physiological and cognitive regulatory systems: Infant sleep regulation and subsequent executive functioning. Child Dev. 2010;81:1739–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01507.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Goodlin-Jones BL, Tang K, Liu J, Anders TF. Sleep patterns in preschoolage children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47:930–8. doi: 10.1097/CHI.ObO13e3181799f7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Polimeni MA, Richdale AL, Francis AJ. A survey of sleep problems in autism, Asperger's disorder and typically developing children. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2005;49:260–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00642.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Corkum P, Tannock R, Moldofsky H. Sleep disturbances in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Academy Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37:637–46. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199806000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wiggs L, Montgomery P, Stores G. Actigraphic and parent reports of sleep patterns and sleep disorders in children with subtypes of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sleep. 2005;28:1437–45. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.11.1437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Richdale AL, Schreck KA. Sleep problems in autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, nature, & possible biopsychosocial aetiologies. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13:403–11. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Golan N, Shahar E, Ravid S, Pillar G. Sleep disorders and daytime sleepiness in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Sleep. 2004;27:261–6. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lecendreaux M, Konofal E, Bouvard M, Falissard B, Mouren-Simeoni M. Sleep and alertness in children with ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:803–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Corkum P, Tannock R, Moldofsky H, Hogg-Johnson S, Humphries T. Actigraphy and parental ratings of sleep in children with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Sleep. 2001;24:303–12. doi: 10.1093/sleep/24.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tsai F-J, Chiang H-L, Lee C-M, et al. Sleep problems in children with autism, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and epilepsy. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6:413–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gruber R, Grizenko N, Schwartz G, Bellingham J, Guzman R, Joober R. Performance on the continuous performance test in children with ADHD is associated with sleep efficiency. Sleep. 2007;30:1003–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.8.1003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gruber R. Impact of sleep restriction on neurobehavioral functioning of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sleep. 2010;34:315–23. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.3.315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Brown CA, Kuo M, Phillips L, Berry R, Tan M. Non-pharmacological sleep interventions for youth with chronic health conditions: A critical review of the methodological quality of the evidence. Disabil Rehabil. 2012 Oct 10; doi: 10.3109/09638288.2012.723788. [epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kuhn B, Elliot AJ. Treatment efficacy in behavioral pediatric sleep medicine. J Psychosom Res. 2003;54:587–97. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(03)00061-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer LJ, Sadeh A. Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night wakings in young children. Sleep. 2006;29:1263–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Owens JL, France KG, Wiggs L. Behavioural and cognitive-behavioural interventions for sleep disorders in infants and children: A review. Sleep Med Rev. 1999;3:281–302. doi: 10.1053/smrv.1999.0082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ramachandani P. A systematic review of treatments for settling problems and night waking in young children. Behav Med. 2009;320:209–13. doi: 10.1136/bmj.320.7229.209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Minde K, Faucon A, Falkner S. Sleep problems in toddlers; Effects of treatment on their daytime behavior. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33:1114–21. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199410000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Owens JA, Palermo TM, Rosen CL. Overview of current management of sleep disturbances in children: II--behavioral interventions. Curr Ther Res. 2002;63(Supplement B):B38–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mindell JA. Empirically supported treatments in pediatric psychology: Bedtime refusal and night wakings in young children. J Pediatr Psychol. 1999;24:465–81. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/24.6.465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mindell JA, Moore M. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2012. The impact of behavioral interventions for sleep problems on secondary outcomes in young children and their families. Oxford handbook of infant, child, and adolescent sleep: development and problems. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Sadeh A. Cognitive-behavioral treatment for childhood sleep disorders. Clin Psychol Rev. 2005;25:612–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Anders TF, Dahl RE. Classifying sleep disorders in infants and toddlers. In: Narrow WE, First MB, Sirovatka PJ, editors. Age and gender considerations in psychiatric diagnosis. Arlington: American Psychiatric Association; 2007. pp. 215–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Scholle S, Beyer U, Bernhard M, et al. Normative values of polysomnographic parameters in childhood and adolescence: Quantitative sleep parameters. Sleep Med. 2011;12:542–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN, Dement WC. The ontogenic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science. 1966;152:604–19. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jenni OG, Dahl RE. Sleep, cognition, and emotion: A developmental view. In: Nelson CA, Luciana M, editors. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2 ed. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2008. pp. 807–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Siegel JM. Clues to the functions of mamalian sleep. Nature. 2005;437:1264–71. doi: 10.1038/nature04285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Jenni OG, Fuhrer HA, Iglowstein I, Molinari L, Largo RH. A longitudinal study of bed sharing and sleep problems among Swiss children in the first 10 years of life. Pediatrics. 2005;115(Suppl 1):233–40. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0815E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Simard V, Nielsen TA, Tremblay RE, Bovin M, Montplaisir JY. Longitudinal study of preschool sleep disturbance: The predictive role of mal-adaptive parental behaviors, early sleep problems, and child/mother psychological factors. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162:360–7. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.162.4.360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Thome M, Skuladottir A. Changes in sleep problems, parent distress and impact of sleep problems from infancy to preschool age for referred and unreferred children. Scand J Caring Sci. 2005;19:86–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2005.00322.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Zelazo PD, Reznick JS, Spinazzola J. Representational flexibility and response control in a multistep multilocation search task. Dev Psychol. 1998;34:203–14. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.34.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Zelazo PD. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:297–301. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Mischel W, Moore B. Effects of attention to symbolically presented awards on self control. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1973;28:172–9. doi: 10.1037/h0035716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez ML. Delay of gratification in children. Science. 1989;224:933–8. doi: 10.1126/science.2658056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Zelazo PD, Cunningham WA. Executive function: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford; 2007. pp. 135–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Waxer M, Morton JB. Multiple processes underlying dimensional change card sort performance: A developmental electrophysiological investigation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;23:3267–79. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Morton JB. Understanding genetic, neurophysiological, and experiential influences on the development of executive functioning: the need for developmental models. WIREs Cogn Sci. 2010;1:709–23. doi: 10.1002/wcs.87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, et al. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3586–94. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Tessner KD, Toga AW. Mapping continued brain growth and gray matter density reduction in dorsal frontal cortex: Inverse relationships during postadolescent brain maturation. J Neurosci. 2001;21:8819–29. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-22-08819.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–3. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Chugani HT, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann Neurol. 1987;22:487–97. doi: 10.1002/ana.410220408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1997;387:167–78. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:475–83. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01633-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Morton JB, Bomsma R, Ansari D. Age-related changes in brain activation associated with dimensional shifts in attention: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2009;46:249–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:115–6. doi: 10.1038/nn1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Milner B. Effects of different brain lesions on Card Sorting: The role of the frontal lobes. Arch Neurol. 1963;9:90–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Dahl RE. The regulation of sleep and arousal: Development and psycho-pathology. Dev Psychopathol. 1996;8:3–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Horne J. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour implications for the pre-frontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:413–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.3.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Beebe DW, Gozal D. Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: Towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res. 2002;11:1–16. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00289.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.O'Hearn K, Asato M, Ordaz S, Luna B. Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1103–32. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Hill E. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Tannock R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1998;39:65–99. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Narzisi A, Muratori F, Calderoni S, Fabbro F, Urgesi C. Neuropsychological profile in high functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012 Dec 8; doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1736-0. [epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Pennington BF, Ozonoff S. Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996;32:1081–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Epsy KA, Sheffield TD, Wiebe SA, Clark CAC, Moehr MJ. Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:33–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02265.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Sonuga-Barke EJS, Dalen L, Daley D, Remington R. Are planning, working memory, and inhibition associated with individual differences in pre-school ADHD symptoms? Dev Neuropsychol. 2002;21:255–72. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2103_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Miyamoto H, Hensch TK. Reciprocal interaction of sleep and synaptic plasticity. Mol Interv. 2003;3:404–17. doi: 10.1124/mi.3.7.404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Walker MP. Sleep, memory and plasticity. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;57:139–66. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Jan JE, Reiter RJ, Bax MCO, Ribary U, Freeman RD, Wasdell MB. Long-term sleep disturbances in children: A cause of neuronal loss. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2010;14:380–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2010.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Huttenlocher PR. Neural plasticity: The effects of environment on the development of the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Rutter M, Sroufe AL. Developmental psychopathlogy: concepts and challenges. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12:265–96. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;28:595–616. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Davidson MC, Amso D, Anderson LC, Diamond A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2037–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Garon N, Moore C. Awareness and symbol use improves future-oriented decision making in preschoolers. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31:39–59. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn3101_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Berger RH, Miller AL, Seifer R, Cares SR, LeBourgeois MK. Acute sleep restriction effects on emotional responses in 30 to 36-month-old children. J Sleep Res. 2012;21:235–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00962.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Horne JA. Human sleep, sleep loss and behaviour. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:413–9. doi: 10.1192/bjp.162.3.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Jones K, Harrison Y. Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5:463–75. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Giannotti F, Cortesi F. Family and cultural influences on sleep development. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2009;18:849–61. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Milan S, Snow S, Belay S. The context of preschool children's sleep: Racial/ethnic differences in sleep locations, routines, and concerns. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21:20–8. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Gregory AM, Eley TC, O'Connor TG, Rijisdijk FV, Plomin R. Family influence on the association between sleep problems and anxiety in a large sample of pre-school aged twins. Pers Individ Diff. 2005;39:1337–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Anders TF. Infant sleep, nighttime relationships, and attachment. Psychiatry. 1994;57:11–21. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1994.11024664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Hale L, Berger LM, LeBourgeois MK, Brooks-Gunn J. Social and demographic predictors of preschoolers' bedtime routines. J Dev Beh Pediatr. 2009;30:394–402. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ba0e64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Burnham MM, Goodlin-Jones BL, Gaylor EE, Anders TF. Nighttime sleep-wake patterns and self-soothing from birth to one year of age: A longitudinal intervention study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43:713–25. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Kataria S, Swanson MS, Trevathan GE. Persistence of sleep disturbances in preschool children. J Pediatr. 1987;110:642–6. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(87)80571-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Matricciani EL, Olds T, Petkov J. In search of lost sleep: Secular trends in the sleep timing of school-aged children and adolescents. Sleep Med Rev. 2012;16:203–11. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Thompson DA, Christakis DA. The association between television viewing and irregular sleep schedules among children less than 3 years of age. Pediatrics. 2005;116:851–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES